dictionary_of_linguistics.pdf

(130 KB) Pobierz
72013156 UNPDF
Dictionary of Linguistics
Table of Contents
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
General features of language
Phonetics and phonology
Morphology
Lexicology
Syntax
Semantics and pragmatics
Sociolinguistics
Psycholinguistics
Applied linguistics
Language change
Language typology
Page
2
7
13
16
18
24
27
30
31
32
35
Amitabh Vikram Dwivedi Dictionary of Linguistics Page 2 of 36
1 General features of language
applied linguistics The application of insights from theoretical linguistics to
practical matters
such as language teaching, remedial linguistic therapy, language planning or
whatever.
arbitrariness An essential notion in structural linguistics which denies any
necessary
relationship between linguistic signs and their referents, e.g. objects in the
outside world.
areas of linguistics Any of a number of areas of study in which linguistic
insights have been
brought to bear, for instance sociolinguistics in which scholars study society and
the way
language is used in it. Other examples are psycholinguistics which is concerned
with the
psychological and linguistic development of the child.
competence According to Chomsky in his Aspects of the theory of syntax (1965) this
is the
abstract ability of an individual to speak the language which he/she has learned
as native
language in his/her childhood. The competence of a speaker is unaffected by such
factors as
nervousness, temporary loss of memory, speech errors, etc. These latter phenomena
are
entirely within the domain of performance which refers to the process of applying
one’s
competence in the act of speaking. Bear in mind that competence also refers to the
ability to
judge if a sentence is grammatically well-formed; it is an unconscious ability.
context A term referring to the environment in which an element (sound, word,
phrase)
occurs. The context may determine what elements may be present, in which case one
says that
there are ‘co-occurrence restrictions’ for instance 1) /r/ may not occur after /s/
in a syllable in
English, e.g. */sri:n/ is not phonotactically permissible in English; 2) the
progressive form
cannot occur with stative verbs, e.g. We are knowing German is not well-formed in
English.
contrast A difference between two linguistic items which can be exploited
systematically.
The distinction between the two forms arises from the fact that these can occupy
one and the
same slot in a syntagm, i.e. they alternate paradigmatically, e.g. the different
inflectional
forms of verbs contrast in both English and German. Forms which contrast are
called
distinctive. This can apply to sounds as well, for instance /p/ and /b/ contrast
in English as
minimal pairs such as pin /pin/ : bin /bin/ show.
convention An agreement, usually reached unconsciously by speakers in a community,
that
relationships are to apply between linguistic items, between these and the outside
world or to
apply in the use of rules in the grammar of their language.
creativity An accepted feature of human language — deriving from the phenomenon of
sentence generation — which accounts for speakers’ ability to produce and to
understand a
theoretically infinite number of sentences.
descriptive An approach to linguistics which is concerned with saying what
language is like
and not what it should be like (prescriptivism).
Amitabh Vikram Dwivedi Dictionary of Linguistics Page 3 of 36
diachronic Refers to language viewed over time and contrasts with synchronic which
refers
to a point in time. This is one of the major structural distinctions introduced by
Saussure and
which is used to characterise types of linguistic investigation.
displacement One of the key characteristics of human language which enables it to
refer to
situations which are not here and now, e.g. I studied linguistics in London when I
was in my
twenties.
duality of patterning A structural principle of human language whereby larger
units consist
of smaller building blocks, the number of such blocks being limited but the
combinations
being almost infinite. For instance all words consist of combinations of a limited
number of
sounds, say about 40 in either English or German. Equally all sentences consist of
structures
from a small set with different words occupying different points in the structures
allowing for
virtually unlimited variety.
economy A principle of linguistic analysis which demands that rules and units are
to be kept
to a minimum, i.e. every postulated rule or unit must be justified linguistically
by capturing a
generalisation about the language being analysed, if not about all languages.
extralinguistic Any phenomenon which lies outside of language. An extralinguistic
reason
for a linguistic feature would be one which is not to be found in the language
itself.
figurative Any use of a word in a non-literal sense, e.g. at the foot of the
mountain where foot
is employed figuratively to indicate the bottom of the mountain. Figurative usage
is the source
of the second meaning of polysemous words.
formalist An adjective referring to linguistic analyses which lay emphasis on
relatively
abstract conceptions of language structure.
general linguistics A broad term for investigations which are concerned with the
nature of
language, procedures of linguistic analysis, etc. without considering to what use
these can be
put. It contrasts explicitly with applied linguistics.
generative A reference to a type of linguistic analysis which relies heavily on
the formulation
of rules for the exhaustive description (generation) of the sentences of a
language.
head The centre of a phrase or sentence which is possibly qualified by further
optional
elements, in the phrase these bright new signs the head is signs as all other
elements refer to it
and are optional. The term is also used in lexicology to refer to the determining
section of a
compound; in family tree, the element tree is head and family is modifier. This
has
consequences for grammar, especially in synthetic languages, such as German where
in a
compound like Stammbuch the gender is neuter (with das) because the head Buch is
although
the modifying word is masculine (der Stamm).
hierarchy Any order of elements from the most central or basic to the most
peripheral, e.g. a
Amitabh Vikram Dwivedi Dictionary of Linguistics Page 4 of 36
hierarchy of word classes in English would include nouns and verbs at the top and
elements
like adjectives and adverbs further down with conjunctions and subordinators still
further
down. The notions of top and bottom are intended in a metaphorical sense.
idealisation A situation where the linguist chooses to ignore details of language
use for
reasons of greater generalisation.
language A system which consists of a set of symbols (sentences) — realised
phonetically by
sounds — which are used in a regular order to convey a certain meaning. Apart from
these
formal characteristics, definitions of languages tend to highlight other aspects
such as the fact
that language is used regularly by humans and that it has a powerful social
function.
lay speaker A general term to refer to an individual who does not possess
linguistic training
and who can be taken to be largely unaware of the structure of language.
level A reference to a set of recognisible divisions in the structure of natural
language. These
divisions are largely independent of each other and are characterised by rules and
regularities
of organisation. Traditionally five levels are recognised: phonetics, phonology,
morphology,
syntax, semantics. Pragmatics may also be considered as a separate level from
semantics.
Furthermore levels may have subdivisions as is the case with morphology which
falls into
inflectional and derivational morphology (the former is concerned with grammatical
endings
and the latter with processes of word-formation). The term ‘level’ may also be
taken to refer
to divisions within syntax in generative grammar.
linguistics The study of language. As a scientific discipline built on objective
principles,
linguistics did not develop until the beginning of the 19th century. The approach
then was
historical as linguists were mainly concerned with the reconstruction of the Indo-
European
language. With the advent of structuralism at the beginning of the 20th century,
it became
oriented towards viewing language at one point in time. The middle of this century
saw a
radically new approach — known as generative grammar — which stressed our
unconscious
knowledge of language and underlying structures to be found in all languages.
linguistic determinism Refers to the view, propounded by Edward Sapir and Benjamin
Lee
Whorf, that language determines the way in which people think. Also termed the
linguistic
relativity hypothesis.
marked A term used to state that a particular form is statistically unusual or
unexpected in a
certain context. For instance zero plurals in English such as sheep or deer are
marked.
metalanguage The language which is used to discuss language; see also object
language.
metaphor An application of a word to another with which it is figuratively but not
literally
associated, e.g. food for thought. This process is very common in the use of
language and may
lead to changes in grammar as with the verb go in English where its spatial
meaning has come
to be used metaphorically for temporal contexts as in He’s going to learn Russian.
onomastics The linguistic study of names, both personal and place names. This
field is
Amitabh Vikram Dwivedi Dictionary of Linguistics Page 5 of 36
particularly concerned with etymology and with the general historical value of the
information which names offer the linguist.
paradigm The set of forms belonging to a particular word-class or member of a
word-class.
A paradigm can be thought of as a vertical list of forms which can occupy a slot
in a syntagm.
Pronounced [/pærqdaim].
parameter Any aspect of language which can obtain a specific value in a given
language, e.g.
canonical word-order which can have the verb in a declarative sentence either
before the
subject, after the subject or after both subject and object. Contrast principle in
this respect.
performance The actual production of language as opposed to the knowledge about
the
structure of one’s native language which a speaker has internalised during
childhood (see
Competence).
productivity A reference to the extent that a given process is not bound in its
application to a
certain input. For instance the prefixation of re- to verbs in modern English is
productive
because this can be done with practically all verbs, e.g. re-think, re-do, re-
write. The term
also refers — in syntax — to the ability of speakers to produce an unlimited
number of
sentences using a limited set of structures.
psychological reality The extent to which the constructs of linguistic theory can
be taken to
have a basis in the human mind, i.e. to somehow be reflected in human cognitive
structures.
Many linguists are divided on this issue, one extreme claiming that this
requirement of a
theory is not necessary, other saying that it is the ultimate test of any
respectable theory.
reflexiveness The possibility of using language to talk about language; this is
one of its
delimiting characteristics with respect to other communication systems.
rhetoric The technique of speaking effectively in public. Regarded in the past as
an art and
cultivated deliberately.
root 1) In grammar the unalterable core of a word to which all suffixes are added,
e.g. friend
in un-friend-li-ness. 2) In etymology, the earliest form of a word. 3) In
phonetics, the part of
the tongue which lies furthest back in the mouth.
Sapir-Whorf hypothesis The notion that thought is determined by language. While
few
linguists nowadays accept this strict link, there would seem to be some truth to
the postulation
of the two American anthropologists/linguists.
sign language A communication system in which people use their hands to convey
signals. In
recent years sign language has been the object of linguists’ attention and has
come to be
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin