psychopathyparalimbic.pdf

(421 KB) Pobierz
doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.013
Psychiatry Research 142 (2006) 107–128
www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres
Review article
A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy:
Evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction
Kent A. Kiehl *
Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Institute of Living, 200 Retreat Ave, Hartford, CT, 06106, USA
Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA
Received 18 May 2005; received in revised form 1 September 2005; accepted 12 September 2005
Abstract
Psychopathy is a complex personality disorder that includes interpersonal and affective traits such as glibness, lack of
empathy, guilt or remorse, shallow affect, and irresponsibility, and behavioral characteristics such as impulsivity, poor
behavioral control, and promiscuity. Much is known about the assessment of psychopathy; however, relatively little is
understood about the relevant brain disturbances. The present review integrates data from studies of behavioral and cognitive
changes associated with focal brain lesions or insults and results from psychophysiology, cognitive psychology and cognitive
and affective neuroscience in health and psychopathy. The review illustrates that the brain regions implicated in psychopathy
include the orbital frontal cortex, insula, anterior and posterior cingulate, amygdala, parahippocampal gyrus, and anterior
superior temporal gyrus. The relevant functional neuroanatomy of psychopathy thus includes limbic and paralimbic structures
that may be collectively termed d the paralimbic system T . The paralimbic system dysfunction model of psychopathy is discussed
as it relates to the extant literature on psychopathy.
D 2006 Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd.
Keywords: Psychopathy; Cognitive neuroscience; Affective neuroscience; Review; fMRI; ERP
Contents
1. Introduction.................................................... 108
2. The construct and assessment of psychopathy .................................. 108
3. Neurology and psychopathy ........................................... 110
4. Neuropsychological test findings in psychopathy................................. 113
5. Abnormalities in neurocognitive function in psychopathy ............................ 113
6. Language processes and psychopathy ...................................... 113
* Tel.: +1 860 545 7385; fax: +1 860 545 7797.
E-mail address: kent.kiehl@yale.edu .
0165-1781/$ - see front matter D 2006 Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd.
427219604.001.png
108
K.A. Kiehl / Psychiatry Research 142 (2006) 107–128
7. Attention and orienting processes in psychopaths ................................. 116
8. Affective processes in psychopathy ........................................ 120
9. Psychopathy as a disorder of the paralimbic system ............................... 122
Acknowledgements ................................................... 124
References ....................................................... 124
1. Introduction
psychotic symptoms or defects in intellectual func-
tion. In the 200 years that followed, the condition has
been through an evolution in terminology, but many
of the defining characteristics have remained un-
changed. These characteristics were most clearly
delineated, and the current diagnostic criteria estab-
lished, by the writings of the psychiatrist Hervey
Cleckley (1941) . In his 40 years of clinical work,
Cleckley came to narrow the syndrome he called
psychopathy to 16 characteristics. Affectively, psy-
chopaths are callous, shallow, and superficial, and
they lack insight and empathy for the effect their poor
behavior has on others; behaviorally, psychopaths are
impulsive, nomadic, and have weak behavioral
control.
In subsequent years, Hare and colleagues oper-
ationalized and transformed Cleckley’s characteristics
into items on the Hare Psychopathy Checklist ( Hare,
1980 ) and its successor, the Hare Psychopathy
Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; Hare, 1991, 2003 ) . The
PCL-R is now the most widely accepted diagnostic
instrument for psychopathy. There is a substantial
literature attesting to the reliability and validity of the
PCL-R as a measure of psychopathy in incarcerated
offenders, forensic patients, psychiatric patients, and
substance abuse patients (see Hare, 2003 , fora
review). The items on the PCL-R are listed in Table 1 .
The PCL-R assessment is performed by reviewing
the participant’s institutional records, including intake
assessments, social worker assessments, and reports
of institutional adjustment and transgressions. A semi-
structured interview covering school adjustment,
employment, intimate relationships, family, friends,
and criminal activity is conducted. Explicit criteria
detailing each item are reviewed from the PCL-R
manual, and each item is scored on a three point scale:
0—does not apply, 1—applies somewhat, and 2—
definitely applies, to the individual. The resulting
scores range from 0 to 40, and the recommended
diagnostic cutoff for psychopathy is 30 ( Hare, 1991,
2003 ). Interviews are typically video taped so that an
Psychopathy is a serious mental health disorder.
Psychopathy is believed to affect approximately 1%
of the general population, 15–25% of the male and
female prison population ( Hare, 1991, 2003 ) , and 10–
15% of substance abuse populations ( Alterman and
Cacciola, 1991; Alterman et al., 1993, 1998 ). Over the
last 20 years, much has been learned about the
assessment and characterization of the forensic and
legal aspects of psychopathy. However, compared
with other psychiatric disorders of similar prevalence,
relatively little is known about the neural systems
implicated in psychopathy. This review will draw on
information from multiple disciplines, including
neurology, psychiatry, psychology, cognitive neuro-
science, psychophysiology, and epileptology. The
literatures will be integrated and a new model of the
functional neuroanatomy underlying psychopathy will
be presented. The review is offered in several parts.
First, the assessment and classification of psychopathy
is reviewed. The second part of the review will draw
upon indirect evidence from studies of how insults or
damage to regions of the brain may lead to symptoms
and cognitive abnormalities consistent with those
observed in psychopathy suggesting that these latter
circuits may be implicated in the disorder. The third
part of the review focuses on the cognitive and
affective neuroscience studies of psychopathy. Finally,
a new model of the functional neuroanatomy under-
lying psychopathy will be presented.
2. The construct and assessment of psychopathy
The modern concept of psychopathy can be traced
back to the psychiatrist Pinel (1792 as cited in
Cleckley, 1941 ) , who labeled the condition d madness
without delirium T . This term was used to denote the
lack of morality and behavioral control in these
individuals that occurred despite the absence of any
K.A. Kiehl / Psychiatry Research 142 (2006) 107–128
109
Four-factor
model
1 Glibness/superficial charm 1 1
2 Grandiose sense of self-worth 1 1
3 Need for stimulation 2 3
4 Pathological lying 1 1
5 Conning/manipulative 1 1
6 Lack of remorse or guilt 1 2
7 Shallow affect 1 2
8 Callous/lack of empathy 1 2
9 Parasitic lifestyle 2 3
10 Poor behavioral controls 2 4
11 Promiscuous sexual behavior – –
12 Early behavioral problems 2 4
13 Lack of realistic, long-term goals 2 3
14 Impulsivity 2 3
15 Irresponsibility 2 3
16 Failure to accept responsibility 1 2
17 Many marital relationships – –
18 Juvenile delinquency 2 4
19 Revocation of conditional release 2 4
20 Criminal versatility – 4
The items corresponding to the early two-factor conceptualization
of psychopathy ( Harpur et al., 1988, 1989 ) and current four-factor
model are listed ( Hare, 2003 ) . The two-factor model labels are
Interpersonal/Affective (Factor 1) and Social Deviance (Factor 2)
and the four-factor model labels are Interpersonal (Factor 1),
Affective (Factor 2), Lifestyle (Factor 3), and Antisocial (Factor 4).
Items followed by a dash did not load on any factor.
grounds of specificity in forensic populations ( Hart
and Hare, 1996 ). Nearly 80–90% of inmates in a
maximum security prison fulfill the criteria for ASPD,
while only 15–25% score above the diagnostic
criteria for psychopathy. In addition, the diagnostic
confusion of psychopathy as putatively measured by
ASPD and the complex relationships of these con-
structs with substance disorders have led to consid-
erable confusion and hampered research efforts. It is
important to stress that proper psychometric assess-
ment of a condition is crucial, particularly when
trying to relate it to psychophysiological or cognitive
neuroscience data. The review below will largely be
constrained to studies that employed psychometric
measures consistent with the classic conceptualization
of psychopathy. That is, the preferred conceptualiza-
tion of psychopathy is a disorder that includes both
interpersonal and affective characteristics and deviant
behavior.
Recent psychometric analyses, using Item Re-
sponse Theory (IRT), of PCL-R scores from large
diverse multicultural samples have revealed a two-
factor four-facet model (see Table 1 ; Hare, 2003 ) .
This model includes facets labeled, Interpersonal
(Factor 1), Affective (Factor 2), Lifestyle (Factor 3),
and Antisocial (Factor 4) (but see also Cooke and
Michie, 1997 , for a three-factor model). There have
been few studies that have examined the neuro-
cognitive correlates of the factor models of psychop-
Soderstrom et al., 2002; Sutton et al., 2002 ).
However, as in complex disorders such as schizo-
phrenia, relating specific symptom clusters to patterns
of cerebral activity may clarify the relevant brain
disturbances and help reconcile discrepant findings
( Liddle, 2001 ) . It may be fruitful therefore to relate
psychophysiological and cognitive neuroscience
measures to the factors of psychopathy. A potential
problem with this method, however, is that the factors
of psychopathy are often strongly correlated, making
it difficult to relate neural correlates to the symptom
profiles.
In summary, research over the last 20 years has
made great strides in characterizing the psychometric
assessment and classification of psychopathy. The
classic conceptualization of psychopathy is a disorder
that includes both interpersonal and affective charac-
teristics and behavioral traits.
independent rating can be obtained. The total assess-
ment time typically ranges from 2 to 5 h, it is rigorous,
and training is required.
Early factor analyses of the PCL-R items revealed
two correlated factors (see Table 1 ; Harpur et al.,
1988, 1989 ). Factor 1 included items related to
emotional and interpersonal relationships. Factor 2
items reflected impulsive and antisocial behaviors.
This latter factor is most closely related to the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
(DSM-IV) classification of Antisocial Personality
1994 ). It is important to note that ASPD has been
criticized for overly relying on antisocial behaviors,
while excluding many of the affective and interper-
sonal characteristics considered to be central to the
construct of psychopathy ( Alterman et al., 1998; Hare,
1996; Hare et al., 1991; Hart and Hare, 1996; Widiger
et al., 1996 ). ASPD also has been questioned on
Table 1
The 20 items listed on the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised ( Hare,
1991, 2003 )
Item
Two-factor
model
427219604.002.png
110
K.A. Kiehl / Psychiatry Research 142 (2006) 107–128
3. Neurology and psychopathy
aggression — a cardinal feature of psychopathy ( Blair,
2001; Hare, 1993 ). Orbital frontal patients also do not
typically exhibit the callousness commonly observed
in psychopathic individuals. Similarly, patients with
d acquired sociopathy T , unlike psychopathic individu-
als, are characterized by lack of motivation, hoarding
behavior, mood disturbances, incontinence, and fail-
ure or inability to make long-term plans ( Blumer and
Benson, 1975 ). Psychopathic individuals, on the other
hand, often enjoy making grandiose life plans — they
just fail to follow through with them.
Patients with orbital frontal lesions show impair-
ment on affective voice and face expression identifi-
cation tasks ( Hornak et al., 1996, 2003 ) , response
reversal or extinction ( Blair and Cipolotti, 2000; Rolls
et al., 1994 ), and decision-making ( Bechara et al.,
2000 ). The animal literature shows that lesions to
orbital frontal cortex impair inhibitory performance on
No Go trials of Go/No Go paradigms ( Iversen and
Mishkin, 1970 ). Psychopaths have problems with
processing certain aspects of affective speech and face
et al., 1998 ). Psychopaths, under certain contextual
demands, also show poor response inhibition ( Kiehl,
2000; Lapierre et al., 1995 ), response modulation
1992; Newman and Schmitt, 1998 ) and more recently
response reversal ( Mitchell et al., 2002 ) . Boys with
psychopathic tendencies and adult psychopaths tend
to show impairments on the Bechara gambling test of
though not all studies have found this effect ( Schmitt
et al., 1999 ).
In summary, damage to the orbital frontal cortex
appears to be associated with some symptoms and
cognitive impairments that may also be found in
psychopaths. However, despite these apparent similar-
ities, no studies have actually explicitly investigated
how orbital frontal patients score on psychopathy
measures. Similarly, many of the studies of cognitive
function in psychopathy and orbital frontal patients
have used different cognitive tests, making inferences
between studies problematic. It is only recently that
identical tasks (i.e., response reversal) have been
examined in both orbital frontal patients and in
samples of psychopathic individuals ( Mitchell et al.,
2002 ). Nevertheless, it appears that lesions to orbital
frontal cortex appear to elicit behaviors that most
One way to examine the possible neural regions
implicated in psychopathic behavior is to draw from
studies of behavioral changes and cognitive impair-
ments associated with damage to specific brain circuits.
It is important to recognize that this method provides
only indirect evidence of the possible regions impli-
cated in psychopathic symptomatology; nevertheless,
some interesting and compelling data have accumulat-
ed. The most notable neurological case study is that of
the railroad worker Phineas Gage ( Harlow, 1848 ) .
Gage suffered penetrating trauma to the prefrontal
cortex ( Damasio et al., 1994 ) . He was transformed by
this accident from a responsible railroad manager to an
impulsive, irresponsible, sexually promiscuous, ver-
bally abusive individual ( Harlow, 1848 ) . Many of
Gage’s symptoms are consistent with those classically
associated with psychopathy.
Subsequent studies of patients with prefrontal lobe
damage suggest that the orbital frontal cortex plays a
role in mediating some behaviors related to psychop-
Damage to the orbital frontal cortex leads to a
condition termed d pseudopsychopathy T ( Blumer and
Benson, 1975 )or d acquired sociopathic personality T
( Damasio, 1994 ) characterized by problems with
reactive aggression, motivation, empathy, planning
and organization, impulsivity, irresponsibility, insight,
and behavioral inhibition ( Malloy et al., 1993; Stuss et
al., 1983 ). In some cases, patients may become prone
to grandiosity ( Blumer and Benson, 1975 ) and
Recent studies suggest that bilateral damage to orbital
frontal cortex is necessary to elicit changes in social
behavior ( Hornak et al., 2003 ) . Moreover, patients with
gross, extensive damage due to stroke or closed head
injury are more likely to exhibit d acquired sociopathic T
symptomology than are patients with focal, circum-
scribed surgical lesions to orbital frontal cortex
( Hornak et al., 2003 ) . These data suggest that some
aspects of psychopathic symptomatology may map
onto the (dys)function of orbital frontal cortex and
adjacent regions. However, the d pseudopsychopathy T
or d acquired sociopathy T model does not appear to fully
account for the constellation of symptoms observed in
psychopathy. For example, patients with orbital frontal
damage rarely show instrumental or goal-directed
K.A. Kiehl / Psychiatry Research 142 (2006) 107–128
111
consistently map onto the Affective (Factor 2) and
Lifestyle (Factor 3) factors of the four-factor psychop-
athy model. These factors of psychopathy include
symptoms of impulsivity, irresponsibility, and stimu-
lation seeking as well as a general lack of empathy. In
some cases, orbital frontal patients’ symptomatology
may be similar to the Interpersonal items (Factor 1),
which include superficial charm, grandiose sense of
self-worth, and pathological lying. Thus, while the
d pseudopsychopathy T or d acquired sociopathy T model
appears to mimic some features of psychopathy, the
two disorders differ in many respects. This raises the
possibility that disturbances in brain regions other than
orbital frontal cortex may contribute to psychopathy.
Other brain regions that may be implicated in
psychopathy include the anterior cingulate. The ante-
rior cingulate is a multifaceted complex structure that is
commonly divided into at least two distinct functional
regions ( Devinsky et al., 1995 ) . The rostral aspect,
often termed the d affective T division, is known to be
involved in pain perception and affect regulation ( Bush
et al., 2000 ). The caudal region, termed the d cognitive T
division, is known to be involved with response
conflict, error monitoring, and task switching, among
other processes ( Kiehl et al., 2000a ) . Selective lesions
to the anterior cingulate are rare, but when they do
occur, they tend to be related to emotional unconcern
( Mesulam, 2000 ) , hostility, irresponsibility, and dis-
agreeableness ( Swick, 2003 ) . Recently, Hornak and
colleagues have shown that selective lesions to bilateral
anterior cingulate cortex produce disturbances in
personality functioning similar to those observed in
patients with orbital frontal lesions ( Hornak et al.,
2003 ). In humans, anterior cingulate lesions lead to
perseveration ( Mesulam, 2000 ) , difficulties in affective
face and voice identification ( Hornak et al., 2003 ) , error
Turken, 2002; Turken and Swick, 1999 ) and response
inhibition abnormalities ( Degos et al., 1993; Tekin and
Cummings, 2002 ). Psychopathy has long been associ-
ated with perseveration (see review by Newman,
1964 ), difficulties in identifying some affective face
more recently, error monitoring ( Bates et al., submitted
for publication ) and response inhibition abnormalities
have shown that the volume of the right anterior
cingulate is positively correlated with harm avoidance
( Pujol et al., 2002 ) . Psychopaths are known to score
low on harm-avoidance measures ( Hare, 1991 ) . Thus,
it would appear that bilateral damage to the anterior
cingulate and/or orbital frontal cortex may lead to
symptoms and cognitive impairments similar to those
observed in psychopathy. The facets of psychopathy
that appear to map onto anterior cingulate dysfunction
include the Affective (Factor 2) and Lifestyle (Factor 3)
factors of the four-factor psychopathy model ( Hare,
2003 ). These latter facets include symptoms of lack of
empathy, shallow affect, impulsivity, and irresponsi-
bility. These symptoms appear to be prevalent in
patients with anterior cingulate damage ( Mesulam,
2000; Swick, 2003 ).
In addition to regions of the frontal cortex, regions in
the temporal lobe may be linked to some symptoms of
psychopathy. Damage to the medial temporal lobe in
general ( Kluever and Bucy, 1938, 1939 ) , and neural
tracts that pass through the amygdala in particular
( Aggleton, 1992 ) , have long been associated with
emotional and behavioral changes in monkeys. These
changes include an unnatural propensity for approach
behavior or fearlessness and unusual tameness. Medial
temporal lobe lesioned monkeys also show excessive
fascination with objects, often placing them in the
mouth, and general hyperactivity and hypersexual
activity. This collection of behaviors has been termed
the Kluever–Bucy syndrome. Only a small minority of
humans with damage to bilateral amygdala exhibit the
full manifestation of the Kluever–Bucy syndrome,
while the majority developed less severe emotional
changes ( Adolphs and Tranel, 2000 ) . One case study
reported that bilateral amygdala damage due to
calcification associated with Urbach–Wiethe disease
may be related to mild antisocial behavior, including
rebelliousness, disregard for social convention, and
lack of respect for authorities ( Adolphs and Tranel,
2000 ).
Detailed psychological and personality assess-
ments of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy suggests
a high incidence of psychopathic-like behavior.
Indeed, some studies have reported that pre-opera-
tively the prevalence of psychopathic-like behaviors is
as high as 70% of patients with anterior temporal lobe
epilepsy ( Blumer, 1975; Hill et al., 1957 ) . Structures
that are commonly implicated in temporal lobe
epilepsy include the amygdala, hippocampus, para-
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin