The-psychobiology-of-aggression-and-violence-Bioethical-implications_2010_International-Social-Science-Journal.pdf

(210 KB) Pobierz
The psychobiology of aggression and violence: bioethical implications
The psychobiology of aggression and
violence: bioethical implications
Jose´ Luis Dı´ az
Introduction
problem of determining whether aggressive
behaviour is innate or acquired. This is a deli-
cate issue because, were there to be a genetic
and biological cause of aggression, it would be
difficult to change this through social learning
and we would be irredeemably condemned to
violence. This view was heavily criticised by the
Seville Statement (Adams 1991), with solid
scientific arguments be difficult stating that,
far from implying that aggression or violence
were genetically deter-
mined, the behavioural,
cognitive and neurological
sciences showed that biolo-
gical determinism is much
less prevalent, which not
only allows but obliges
us to consider social ele-
ments as necessarily rele-
vant in their processing and
expression.
This study is a sum-
mary of certain psycho-
biology research topics
that are relevant to aggres-
sion and violence and to
the development of bio-
ethical arguments. Special
attention is given to the
problem of distinguishing
innate from acquired
aspects of aggressive beha-
viour, the ethological understanding and defini-
tion of aggression, the biological basis for this
behaviour and the link between emotions and
aggression. The aim is for this platform to be
used eventually to develop a bioethical argument
Bioethics is concerned with the ethical andmoral
aspects of life-related phenomena covered by
biology and medicine. Topics such as the use of
animals in harmful experiments, human respon-
sibility within ecosystems, abortion, euthanasia
or the use of stem cells for scientific and
therapeutic purposes have been widely analysed
and debated. Potentially, the field is very wide
and can be applied in other
socially relevant ways. One of
these is the bioethical implica-
tions of cognitive and beha-
vioural science, particularly
the subject of aggression and
violence. The relevance of this
subject is clear, as very differ-
ent moral and legal responsi-
bilities may apply depending
on whether aggression and
violence are forms of beha-
viour that are innate or
acquired, deliberate or auto-
matic, understandable and
justifiable based on causes,
or how they relate to certain
neurological and psychiatric
conditions. In this and the
above-mentioned topics, bio-
logical research and natural
science theories are basic
ingredients for reflections, arguments and deci-
sions concerning ethics.
One of the original and recurring themes of
analysis in behavioural science (in terms of the
social role of aggression) is the apparent
Jose´ Luis Dı´ az is in the Department of the
History and Philosophy of Medicine,
Faculty of Medicine, National Autono-
mous University of Mexico. The main
focus of his research interests is psycho-
biology or the study of the biological and
cerebral basis of the mind and behaviour.
His studies have covered neurochemistry,
psychopharmacology, ethnopharmacol-
ogy, ethology, the mind–body problem
the nature of consciousness, cognitive
science and epistemology. His recent
publications include H. Vargas-Pe´ rez, L.
Sellings, T. Grieder and J.L. Dı´ az, (2009)
Social dominance rank influences wheel-
running behaviour in mice in Neuroscience
Letters, 457 (3), 137–140; J.L. Dı´ az, (2007)
La conciencia viviente.M´ xico: Fondo de
Cultura Econo´ mica; and J.L. Dı´ az (2009)
The legacy of Cajal inMexico in Revista de
Neurologı ´ a, 48 (4), 207–215.
ISSJ 200–201 r UNESCO2011. Published byBlackwell PublishingLtd., 9600GarsingtonRoad, Oxford, OX4 2DK, UKand 350Main Street, Malden,MA02148, USA.
784983425.002.png
234
Jose´ Luis Dı´az
founded on an empirical basis. The study does
not tackle the issue of genetic influence on
aggressive human behaviour, as this subject has
been widely analysed by the renowned Nuffield
Council on Bioethics (2002) in its report Genetics
and human behaviour: the ethical context.That
exhaustive and critical report makes it clear that
genetic and acquired factors are involved in the
expression of aggressive behaviour and that it is
methodologically difficult to distinguish between
them. The present study will thus be limited to
certain behavioural, cognitive and physiological
aspects of aggression. Before entering the sub-
stantive part of the study, the concepts involved
must first be defined.
The term violence tends to apply to any
event that occurs with unusual force, such as a
typhoon, earthquake or train collisions. In terms
of social interactions, we talk of violence when
the following two conditions are fulfilled: the use
or application of intense aggression that inflicts
serious damage to people or their property and
the use of this damaging force against what is
considered natural, fair, moral or legal. In both
senses of an attack that disturbs the natural state
and violates a social rule, the application of the
term would appear to be limited to human
beings and a distinction should therefore be
made between violence and aggression in terms
that not all aggression is violent – only attacks that
are harmful or destructive to subjects or objects
and that threaten, weaken or break natural, social
and cultural rules. We will see that certain
incidents in primate groups could be classified as
violent in some of these ways. To locate and
understand the issue of violence, it is therefore
vital to consider the concept and phenomenon of
aggression. Aggression should in turn be analysed
on the basis of at least two elements: a group of
emotions and a group of forms of behaviour. This
distinction is relevant because the emotions of
anger, fury or rage that often precede and
accompany aggression may or may not trigger
behaviour or actions of directed force that risk
producing or do produce pain, injury, fear or
terror in the individual on the receiving end. For
the moment this is the operational definition of
aggressive behaviour that will be the subject of a
critical analysis in this study.
Firstly, the subject of aggression is ad-
dressed in relation to an experimental model
that adequately distinguishes behavioural from
biological causes, which is a relevant issue for
ethics. Secondly, the development of the concept
of aggression in behavioural sciences is exam-
ined. Thirdly, the link between aggressive
behaviour and the emotions that tend to trigger
and accompany it (particularly anger and rage)
is addressed in terms of both phenomenology
and neuropsychology.
Aggression and social
dominance in animals:
regrouping by rank
One way of approaching the origin of complex
forms of behaviour such as violence and aggres-
sion is to use experiments to determine whether
the biological variables precede or follow the
social behaviour. This can be achieved using
various techniques, and one that appears rele-
vant to the purposes of this study is the method
of regrouping male mice by dominance devel-
oped by the author during various studies
carried out in the 1980s. The method consists
in creating groups of three mice in which, within
a few days and with varying degrees and
frequency of conflict, a relatively stable hier-
archy is established with one aggressive and
dominant mouse and two evasive and submis-
sive mice. It is easy to recognise rank during
attacks and fights in the cage by simply
identifying the animal that attacks and the
attacked animal that flees or the winner and
loser in a contest. The behaviour and the actors
can be easily identified, as the repertoire of
attack and evasion behaviour is very well known
in field and laboratory rodents thanks to the
classic work of Scott (1966). To identify the
individuals involved in the agonistic interac-
tions, their back fur is marked with one of three
different colours of permanent ink.
Once the ranking has been established and
stabilised it is possible to carry out various
biological measurements on the animals. How-
ever, measuring a variable does not reveal
whether it is the cause or consequence of the
dominant or subordinate behaviour. In order to
establish causality the mice were regrouped
using their rank to form new groups of three
dominant males and three subordinate males by
mixing animals of previously established and
known ranks. After a few days, new dominances
r UNESCO 2011.
784983425.003.png
Bioethical implications of aggression and violence
235
were established in all groups, giving at least
four combinations of successive ranks:
& dominant in the first and second grouping
(D-D)
& subordinate in the first and second grouping
(S-S)
& dominant first, and then subordinate in the
second grouping (D-S)
& subordinate first, and then dominant in the
second grouping (S-D)
Thus, if a biological variable is measured in
animals in which the history of dominance is
known, it is possible to establish whether it is a
cause or consequence of the rank and the
associated aggression or flight behaviour. The
domination–subordination relationship is estab-
lished mainly through the display of aggressive
and submissive behaviour, which makes it a
social phenomenon that results from the ago-
nistic behaviour and also regulates it.
This strategy was used to establish that
dominant mice have significantly lower cerebral
enkephalin content than the subordinates (Dı ´ az
and Asai, 1990). It is well known that enkepha-
lins are neurotransmitters and modulators
involved in the central nervous mechanisms of
reward and pain. The technique of grouping
mice by rank demonstrated that the methionine-
enkephalin content in the brainstem is much
lower in doubly dominant animals (D-D) than in
the repeatedly subordinate animals (S-S) and
that the level falls dramatically once the
dominant rank is attained by previously sub-
ordinate animals (S-D), while it increases con-
siderably in mice (D-S) that lose the dominant
rank from the first grouping to become
subordinate in the second. It is possible to
conclude that behaviour associated with hier-
archical rank (namely, the aggression and attack
involved in dominance and the submission and
flight involved in subordination) may bring
about significant changes in the content of
neuromodulators related to pleasure and pain
in the brain. One interpretation of the results is
that the neurological system for pain undergoes
a preventive adaptation and coping mechanism
in relation to the stress of injuries associated
with subordination. Indeed, in these experi-
ments there were a high number of injuries
resulting from bites and cuts inflicted upon the
subordinate mice by the dominant animals.
In order to further assess the time dynamics
involved in losing and achieving social domina-
tion, the author carried out other experiments that
were not published at the time. Data from one of
these experiments are provided in Table 1.
Seventy-five 12-week old male albino
BALB-c mice were divided into 25 groups of
three. Every day the fights and attacks and the
winners and losers of each dispute were recorded
for 1 hour. The consistent winners were con-
sidered dominant on the fifth day of consecutive
victories. After 3 weeks, hierarchical social
structures were detected in 21 of the 25 groups,
which meant there were 21 dominant mice each
with two subordinate mice (42 subordinate
animals). In the remaining four groups there
was no aggressive behaviour, attacks or injuries.
On day 22 of the experiment the animals were
regrouped into seven groups of three dominant
mice, 14 groups of three subordinate mice and
four groups of non-aggressive mice (third
column of Table 1). Their behaviour continued
Ta b l e 1. Redistribution of groups of three male BALB/c mice based on dominance hierarchy
First grouping (days 1–22)
Regrouping by rank (days 22–43)
Initial Result Regrouping Result
25 groups of 3 mice 21 hierarchical groups 7 groups of dominants 5 hierarchical groups 5 5 D-D
10 S-S
1 non-aggressive group 3 D-N
1 uncertain group 3 D-U
14 groups of subordinates 5 hierarchical groups 5 S-D
10 S-S
6 non-aggressive groups 18 S-N
3 uncertain groups 9 S-U
4 non-aggressive groups 4 non-aggressive groups 2 non-aggressive groups 6 N-N
2 uncertain groups
Final
distribution
6 N-U
Ranks: D, dominant; S, subordinate; N, non-aggressive; U, uncertain rank.
r UNESCO 2011.
784983425.004.png
236
Jose´ Luis Dı´az
a
Grouping by hierarchy – attack dynamics
b
Time (days)
Figure 1. Time dynamics of attacks in a study involving regrouping based on dominance rank
to be recorded in the same way. In many of the
new groups there was a new social structure with
a dominant mouse and two subordinates. The
results are presented in the last two columns of
Table 1. New ranks were recognised in five of the
seven groups of dominant mice, while in the
other two groups the ranks were uncertain or
there were no fights. In contrast, only five of the
14 subordinate groups showed structures of
dominance. In six groups there were no fights or
attacks, while in the other three groups there
were fights but no winner or dominant mouse
was established. Lastly, in the four groups that
had displayed no aggression in the first round,
two groups engaged in fights but these did not
lead to an identifiably dominant animal, while in
the remaining two groups social calm continued
to reign.
These results of regrouping by rank indicate
considerable variability in the expression of
aggression and submission in mice, despite the fact
that they come from a laboratory strain that is over
99 per cent genetically identical. This variability in
behaviour necessarily implies acquired factors of
an epigenetic, learned or circumstantial nature that
depend on the combination of certain individuals
for a dominant rank to be established with a stable
social structure based on the display of agonistic
aggressive and submissive behaviour.
An analysis of the aggression dynamics in
the groups reveals learned factors in aggression.
Figure 1 shows the time of the experiment along
r UNESCO 2011.
784983425.005.png
Bioethical implications of aggression and violence
237
Total
(74)
No.of mice in each category
Figure 2. Weight gain in dominant (D), subordinate (S) and non-aggressive (N) male mice by dominance rank
showing &, initial grouping (days 1–22) and & regrouping by hierarchy (days 22–43)
the x-axis and the attacks are recorded along
the y-axis. In the first grouping (days 1–20),
there was little aggression on the first day
but it increased and reached its peak on the
fifth day (with an average of 1.4 attacks per hour
for the entire sample and almost six attacks
per hour by the 16 attacking mice). From
that day, the attacks decreased rapidly before
stabilising from day 10, when the groups settled
down into a pattern of one dominant animal and
two subordinates with little detectable aggres-
sion. The attack dynamics were very different
for the grouping introduced from day 21. This
time around, aggression peaked on the first
day, with over two attacks per mouse and 30
contenders attacking nearly six times an hour. In
contrast with the first grouping, dominance ranks
were already established by the second day and
subsequent aggressions dropped to levels lower
than in the previous period.
The Figure 1b shows the daily average of
attacks recorded for the 75 mice of the sample,
while Figure 1a shows average attacks by mice
that displayed aggressive behaviour. Figure 1a
shows that the number of attacking mice is
above average. The vertical bars in each group
represent the standard error.
These data point to several conclusions in
terms of the innate or acquired nature of social
aggression and dominance in mice with practi-
cally identical genomes. The first conclusion is
that this behaviour has a strong learned compo-
nent, demonstrated by the exponential and rapid
aggression dynamic in the groups made up
of experienced animals, compared with the
dynamic in the inexperienced first grouping.
The formation of social structures was much
more rapid and efficient in the second grouping,
and aggression plays a stabilising roles as group
stability is higher when aggression is established
more efficiently and with lower stress and injury
costs in implementing andmaintaining the social
structure.
Another interesting variable was body-
weight, as measured once a week. Figure 2 is a
histogram of gains in bodyweight in grams
r UNESCO 2011.
784983425.001.png
Zgłoś jeśli naruszono regulamin